By Katherine J. Wu April 28, 2022
Maybe that logic seems obvious. Of course dogsâ behavioral quirks, much like those of humans, arenât mere products of genetics or pedigree; of course experiences factor in. Even Brandi Hunter Munden, of the AKC, which details breed personalities on its website, acknowledged to me in an email that âevery dog is different.â And yet, breedâa concept predicated on purity, sameness, predictability passed from parent to pupâis an undeniably powerful force in dogs. âI donât think youâre ever starting from a totally clean slate,â says Gita Gnanadesikan, an evolutionary biologist at the University of Arizona.
Dogs are, in part, âa human creationâthey are not something that existed before us,â says Isain Zapata, of Rocky Vista University, in Colorado, whoâs studied dog genetics and behavior. And over millennia, we have sculpted them to fit a multitude of functions and forms. Purebred canines are a product of human preferences and prejudices; they should be expected to have certain proclivities written into their genes, exactly because humans decided they should. âDoes breed matter? Does breed not matter?â says Kathleen Morrill, a dog geneticist at the Broad Institute and UMass Chan Medical School. âReally, itâs both.â
Experts agree that dog behavior is the product of a multitude of factorsâamong them genes, development, socialization, and environment; they disagree on the ratios, the measurements, the ways in which they swirl together. The key ingredient in every recipe, though, is always us: the people who fancy themselves the arbiters of what makes a dog a dog. The sway of breed, even over personality, is not fictionâour species has made sure of that. But its influence isnât just over dogs, and itâs not as simple as we might like to think.
Many different versions of the dog origin story exist (and more than one may be true), but the gist tends to go like this. Some tens of thousands of years ago, wolves and humans started spending a lot more time together, and began to co-evolve. Itâs not clear who made the first moveâmaybe it was the canines, lured into encampments by their noses; maybe the two species just found themselves thrown together, and bonded over a mutual love for meat. In any case, the chillest and chummiest canines of the bunch kept coming back. At first perhaps lopsided, the relationship soon became more mutually beneficial: People realized that dogs could enhance humansâ ability to feed and protect their families, and eventually, corral their sheep and cattle; the animals would do this in exchange for calories, shelter, and maybe some well-earned belly rubs.
The first chapter of the dog-human relationship, then, was about function. People noticed behaviors they liked in the animals, and started to favor them, âmaybe giving them extra food, giving them a chance to breed,â says Kathryn Lord, a dog-behavior-and-evolution expert at UMass Chan and the Broad Institute, where sheâs working with Morrill. Slowly, a wolfish lineage shed some of its fear of people, and some of its grump; it lost the sharpness of its lupine features, and its apex-predator edge. Even the animalsâ tightly tuned predatory sequenceâsearch, stalk, chase, grab, killâfractured, yielding groups of dogs that specialized in, for instance, stalking and sprinting (herders), pursuing and catching (retrievers), all of the above (terriers), or none of the above (livestock guardians). Under the pressures of employment, dogs diversified.
Then, in the 1800s, dog-rearing underwent a massive pivot. âVictorians changed the way we think about dogs,â says Michael Worboys, a historian of science at the University of Manchester, in England, and an author of The Invention of the Modern Dog: Breed and Blood in Victorian Britain. It was in this periodâthe era of âfancy everything,â as Lord puts itâthat the modern concept of breed was born. Suddenly, people prized dogs more for their looks than the tasks they could perform. Puppeteering the sex lives of dogs became ultra-purposeful, ultra-fashionable; the idea of breed became so valuable that it needed to be policed by stringent criteria and formal clubs. And as the goalposts shifted to achieving purity of blood and physical ideals, canine evolution bent fast. âOnce you start selecting on formâyour coat color, your shape,â Lord told me, âitâs so much more powerful than selecting on what it behaves like.â The number of distinct breeds ballooned, and the dogs within them grew more and more alike.
Nowadays, that uniformity seems a scientific dream: Purebred genomes have been stripped of much of diversityâs noisiness, making patterns within groups easier to spot; with the dog genome sequenced, it should be easy to go in and figure out how human meddling has, at various points, cemented both physical and behavioral propensities into DNA. But behavior is extremely complicatedâsometimes involving many, many genes that may each have only a small influenceâand itâs been repeatedly wrung through humansâ changing ideas about what makes for a very, very good boy.
To untangle the gene-behavior snarl, researchers first need to find lots and lots of dogsâthousands, tens of thousands, the more the betterâto represent enough diversity in both behavior and genetics, and draw connective tissue between them. Morrill, Lord, and their colleagues recently wrapped up one such gargantuan study, one of the most sprawling and in-depth to date. They distributed behavioral surveys to the human companions of roughly 20,000 dogs, asking the same sorts of queries that psychologists use to suss out personality in people, with a canine-focused kick: Does your dog behave fearfully toward unfamiliar people? Cower during storms? Ignore commands? Get pushy with other dogs? They then whole-genome-sequenced the saliva of about 2,000 of those dogs, and searched the DNA for signatures that might help explain the ownersâ answers. Unlike other studies of its ilk, Morrillâs also took care to enroll a lot of muttsâdogs whose appearance and personality are ânaturally shuffled up,â she said.
The teamâs findings confirmed that some aspects of canine behavior do seem quite heritableâand sometimes even echo kennel-club dogma. Work, it turns out, is a pretty good motivator, and several of the traits with genetic ties were probably the ones that kept a lot of early dogs employed. Many herder dogs, for instanceâborder collies and the likeâremain very herder-y. Theyâre still, on average, more likely than other pups to comply with human commands, be curious about their surroundings, and make an enthusiastic bid for toys. Retrievers, too, seem to have fetching written in their genes; it was âthe most heritable behavior we could find in our study,â Morrill said. A few other patterns might be similarly rationalizable: Great Pyrenees, which originated as livestock guardians, tended to be tougher to rattle than other dogs. Beagles, historically tasked with zooming after prey, generally trended toward being headstrong. Centuries of career focus have clearly left a legacy in canine genes. And in at least a few ways, some breeds are still what humans bred them to be.
The further behaviors drift outside the professional arena, though, the trickier they can be to assess, and the trickier their genetic roots are to nail down. Owners might reliably describe how their dogs go bounding after balls, but they might have a less objective sense of whether their canine is especially apt to be calm or skittish, aloof or clingy, assertive or easygoingâall traits that can fall victim to the vagaries of human perception. Scientists seeking a bit more objectivity will sometimes try out laboratory experiments: Someone interested in gauging timidness, for example, can put a dog in a pen with an unfamiliar object, such as an unnervingly plush robotic cat, and watch how much it wigs. But not all behavioral quirks, or dogs, lend themselves to tests in a weird building staffed by strangers, where itâs easy for animals to completely lose their cool. And few researchers are willing to do that thousands of times over for a giant genetic study, already weighed down by time and cost. None of that makes behavior data uselessâjust tougher to interpret, and then straightforwardly explain.
But, other experts told me, with so many dogs banked in their study, Morrill and her colleagues seem to have hit upon some solid connections. And at least as important as any of the genetic trends they found, Morrill said, were the ones they very much did not. In the end, they couldnât find a single behavioral trait that was either absent from any of the breeds they surveyed, or present in every dog within a given breed. Sure, greyhounds, on average, were more blasĂ© about toys than other dogs, while German shepherds seem genetically poised to think theyâre swell. Lots of Chihuahuas are trembly little nubbins, many Brittanys eat their own poop, and Shiba Inus, as a group, will not be the first to jump in your cuddle puddle. All of these, however, are predilections, not prescriptions. There exist border collies who wonât herd, and pugs who will; there exist high-strung Great Pyrenees, and beagles who will obey every command. Breeds may draw loose borders around dog behaviors. But the boundaries are far-flung and poorly patrolledâeasy for an individual to stray outside of, if the conditions are right.
According to Morrillâs team, breed explains just a small fraction of the mind-boggling variation in behavior seen in the species that is dogâless than 10 percent. Which is to say, most of the mishmash can be attributed to something else. That might seem like too small a proportion to some; other, older studies, which analyzed their own data somewhat differently, have made higher estimates. The AKC, unsurprisingly, isnât totally on board with what the new study found. In a statement, the organization reiterated that âbreed and type of dog does inform about general and instinctual behavior,â and said it thinks owners should let those penchants guide decision making. Elinor Karlsson, the computational biologist who led the study, doesnât quite agree. âYou might be able to take a random dog off the street and make a prediction about it based on its breed, and you might be right more often than if it was a totally random guess,â she told me. âBut itâs not going to be particularly effective.â
None of this means that intel on a dogâs breed is worthless. Encoded into breed is still a rich lineage, a history, a wealth of information about how a dogâs body will be built and how it will be set up to navigate its surroundings. Purebred dogs will still, generally, look a certain way. They may even be more likely to behave a certain way. Those propensities just have to contend with the real world, not just once, but over and over and over again. These collisions fascinate Flavio Ayrosa, of the University of SĂŁo Paulo, who has studied how a dogâs height, weight, and nose size, alongside factors such as genes and socialization, might affect its temperament. Small dogs have a different experience of the world than big ones; long snouts bisect a dogâs vision in a way that short schnozzes donât. All of that matters. âThese morphological factors affect how an animal will interact with its environment,â he told me.
Breed information can also help set expectations for what certain dogs might need to stay happy and healthy, and what might be physically feasible for them. Dogs are flexible, but not infinitely so. âYou cannot make a Chihuahua race with greyhounds, or make a Chihuahua a sled dog,â says Carlos Alvarez, a geneticist at Nationwide Childrenâs Hospital, in Ohio. It helps, too, to consider the professional legacies that humans have seeded across the dog family tree. Even allowing for individuality, someone who wants a dog content to stay cooped up in a studio apartment all day might be taking a gamble by adopting one whose ancestors were sprinters and chasers.
The point, then, isnât to discount breedâs influence over dogs, but to rethink its sway over us. People who go after particular dog breeds may do so under the pretense that their new pets will act a certain way. And then they treat them as such, emphasizing and exaggerating the very behaviors they wanted out of their dogs in the first place, while suppressing others. They teach a âcleverâ dog more tricks because they assume that the animal will learn them; they give an âaloofâ dog more space because they figure their pet needs the time alone. Stereotypes become âself-fulfilling prophecies,â Bekoff, of CU Boulder, told me. Dog behaviors are what we breed them to be, but also what we expect them to be. âHow much of how breeds behave is how we behave toward breeds?â Alonso, of the International Association of Animal Behavior Consultants, said. âThat is the million-dollar question.â
The answer, in fact, may be: a decent bit, at least for some dogs. In the UMass-Broad study, purebred golden and Labrador retrievers âtended to score exceptionally,â Karlsson told me, on people-friendly metricsâexactly as the AKC website says they should. But those effects evaporated when her team turned their lens to mutts with retriever ancestry, who are harder to typecast by appearance alone. (Most people, by the way, arenât actually that good at correctly guessing a dogâs lineage.) Even after the researchers accounted for the muttsâ mixed heritage, they found that the part-retrievers werenât any more eager to mingle than the average pooch.
While friendly dogs are considered desirable, the stereotype pendulum swings the opposite way with aggression, a murky label that some behaviorists dislike and yet is often inappropriately pasted onto dogs who then end up banned from housing complexes, abandoned in shelters, even euthanized based on breed alone. Dogs who fall into the pit-bull category are a famous, and particularly controversial, example of this: Bred to fight other animals, theyâve acquired a reputation for violence and unpredictability, a stigma worsened, scholars have argued, by racism against Americaâs urban Black and Latino communities, to which the dogs were culturally linked in the mid-20th century. Some experts argue that caution around pit bulls is warranted, given their history; people who look at pictures of the dogs tend to rate them unfavorably. And yet, studies done by Alvarez, Zapata, and others have found that pit bulls donât seem to be more aggressive or volatile than other dogs. If any dogs are a bit more apt to react when provoked, Alvarez told me, it might be the shrimpy onesâChihuahuas, dachshunds, and so onâperhaps because their teenier brains have a harder time reining in impulsive behaviors ⊠or because theyâre just smaller dogs, constantly being loomed over, picked up, or accidentally kicked.
Plenty of personality descriptors leashed to the dog world might seem far more benign. Theyâre almost horoscopic in natureâsnazzy yet vague and catch-all: Shelties are bright; Boston terriers are amusing; Yorkshire terriers are tomboyish; toy poodles are self-confident; Clumber spaniels are gentlemanly; Chow Chows are serious-minded. Hunter Munden, of the AKC, defended her organizationâs descriptors, explaining that theyâre usually drawn directly from breed standardsâdetailed criteria that lay out the traits for a breedâs âidealâ specimen and include temperament and behaviors that are, she said, âinnate to a breed.â But according to experts such as ĂdĂĄm MiklĂłsi, a dog-cognition researcher at Eötvös LorĂĄnd University, in Hungary, terms like these are ridiculous anthropomorphizations, so squishy that theyâre meaningless, meant more to market dogs than accurately describe them. And they can verge on harmful when they set expectations that canât be met.
Assumptions arenât easy to shake. Even Alonso, who started working with dogs in the late â70s, still hasnât quite broken her âdecades-long habit yet of ascribing certain behavioral traits to breeds,â she told me. She never faults individual dogs for wandering beyond a stereotype. Sheâs just not that surprised when she encounters one who fits its own breed bill: an overprotective Akita, a cattle dog who just wonât stop chasing the kids. âI know itâs wrong,â she told me. âIâm still wrestling with myself.â
Maybe thatâs simply human nature. We buy into the idea that personality is ultra-heritable, because it supports the idea that we made dogs the way they are, that we warped wild wolves into workers, guides, companions, and teammates, with personalities as transparent as Tinder profiles. There is a comfort in the notion that dogs are predictable, categorizable, easy to bin into the boxes we have created for them; that in their behaviors are the motivations and emotions, the University of Manchesterâs Worboys told me, that we humans feel. Many of the descriptive traits we cling most to in dogsâloyal, friendly, lovingâmirror the traits that âwe want to associate with ourselves,â Zapata told me. But dogs are their own animals, as individual as we humans are. Dogs, like humans, can buck the trends of heritage. Dogs, like humans, can shift over a lifetime, trading bad habits for good ones. Dogs, like humans, are âcontinually changing systems, always in development,â Ayrosa, of the University of SĂŁo Paulo, told me, from conception to death. And dogs, like humans, can alter the trajectory of other species, as they have with us.
Alonsoâs beagle, Nellie, scored about as stereotypically beagle as they
come on the surveys used in the UMass-Broad study. But she refuses to be
captured by a single data point. She and Alonso met six years ago, when
Nellie was a âseparation-anxious, resource-guarding, bitey, anxious
dog,â Alonso told meâso riddled with tough-to-take behaviors that sheâd
been adopted, then unadopted, by multiple owners before. Now
Nellieâs fine being left alone at home for several hours at a time. She
never bites. Sheâs welcoming to humans and other dogs alike. Sheâll even
share her food, unless french fries are involved. And she soothed
Alonsoâs pain after her two sons left for college and her two pre-Nellie
dogs died. The pair have shaped each other, exactly as companions would
be expected to do.
- Read: A new origin story for dogs
- đ
- Read: Why so many Millennials are obsessed with dogs
- đ
- Read: All the sad, lonely pandemic puppies
- đ
- Read: Pit bulls are chiller than Chihuahuas
No comments:
Post a Comment